Construction Grammar
- LAST REVIEWED: 20 March 2024
- LAST MODIFIED: 27 March 2014
- DOI: 10.1093/obo/9780199772810-0061
- LAST REVIEWED: 20 March 2024
- LAST MODIFIED: 27 March 2014
- DOI: 10.1093/obo/9780199772810-0061
Introduction
Construction Grammar was originally developed as a monotonic, constraint-based framework whose conceptual basis rests on the fundamental assumption that grammatical patterns are complex signs, in principle not much different from lexical signs: a grammatical pattern is treated as a conventional association between form and function or meaning, i.e., a grammatical construction in a technical, theoretically grounded sense. The framework is the brainchild of Charles Fillmore, who started publishing his constructional research in the mid- to late 1980s. The approach developed out of a confluence of interests—linguistic, cognitive, anthropological, philosophical, computational—that all revolved around the idea that linguistic form is inextricably bound to its meaning and its communicative function and that this connection must be the basis for any descriptively and explanatorily adequate theory of language. Constructions are multidimensional symbolic entities that represent hypotheses about speaker’s linguistic knowledge and, as such, they allow for both the Gestalt view of linguistic patterning (unlike mainstream generative theories of language) and for keeping track of the internal properties of larger patterns (like any other grammatical theory). Constructional analysis thus makes a systematic distinction between what conventionally identifies a construction as a whole versus what is characteristic of its constituents, thereby giving a theoretical status to the well-known slogan that a construction is not just the sum of its parts: it may indeed have its own idiosyncratic properties, unpredictable from the properties of its constituents. Grammar, then, is seen as consisting of networks of constructions, related through shared properties. Constructionally based analysis is now a widely known and widely accepted approach, which continues to develop and extend in various new directions. It is not a monolithic enterprise with uniform goals, interests, or even representational conventions. There are various strands of constructional analysis, some more formally oriented (Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar and its most recent variant, Sign-Based Construction Grammar), some more cognitively focused (usage-based models), not all of them monotonic, not all of them sharing the same formalism. Nevertheless, all constructional scholarship reflects the universal interest of constructional grammarians in studying grammar in its use, rather than as an abstract entity independent of its communicative and cognitive grounding. Different strands overlap in various ways and to a varying degree, but differences among scholars are more a matter of emphasis than a fundamental disagreement about the nature of grammatical structure. The focus of this article is the Fillmorean strand and its most directly related extensions. While originally designed for the purpose of synchronic syntactic descriptions, Construction Grammar has now found its way into many other areas of linguistic research, including morphology, discourse and text analysis, diachrony, acquisition, computational applications, and corpus linguistics. It follows that the field is also developing methodologically, including emphasis on sophisticated statistical methods and some interest in experimental research as contributions toward improving the empirical grounding of constructional analyses.
Introductory Works
Construction Grammar is a relatively new framework, in some areas still developing and in general somewhat fragmented, reflecting various strands of constructional thinking. Consequently, a comprehensive textbook has not yet been written. Fried and Östman 2004 offers a concise overview of the framework and its application in all basic areas of syntactic analysis that can be taken as an interim solution. There are several articles that can serve as a general overview and explanation of the motivation for Construction Grammar, such as Fillmore 1988, Fillmore 1989, or Zwicky 1994.
Fillmore, Charles J. 1988. The mechanisms of “Construction Grammar.” In Proceedings of the fourteenth annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Edited by Shelley Axmaker, Annie Jaisser, and Helen Singmaster, 35–55. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
A brief overview of the constructional approach, walking the reader through several specific issues from English syntax and illustrating the advantages of taking a constructional approach. Accessible to undergraduate readers.
Fillmore, Charles J. 1989. Grammatical Construction: Theory and the familiar dichotomies. In Language processing in social context. Edited by Rainer Dietrich and Carl F. Graumann, 17–38. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Regular scholarly paper, which requires decent familiarity with syntactic theory and the main controversies surrounding the choice between a formal generative versus constructional approach. Extremely useful for understanding construction grammarians’ position vis-à-vis the mainstream generative model of language.
Fried, Mirjam, and Jan-Ola Östman. 2004. Construction Grammar: A thumbnail sketch. In Construction Grammar in a cross-language perspective. Edited by Mirjam Fried and Jan-Ola Östman, 11–86. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
DOI: 10.1075/cal.2
The only relatively comprehensive introduction to Construction Grammar that is available in a published form. A straightforward and reader-friendly overview of the framework, appropriate for undergraduate courses. It introduces the basic concepts, terminology, formalism, and main analytic issues. Examples are taken from various languages and explained in detail. No exercises.
Zwicky, Arnold M. 1994. Dealing out meaning: Fundamentals of syntactic constructions. Proceedings of the twentieth annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistic Society. Edited by Suzanne Gahl, Andy Dolby, and Christopher Johnson, 611–625. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
This early study, though brief and limited in scope, is a useful practical example of the problems that arise in identifying constraints on syntactic patterning and establishing relationships across syntactic constructions. The material is English wh-clauses and their relationship to other English constructions.
Users without a subscription are not able to see the full content on this page. Please subscribe or login.
How to Subscribe
Oxford Bibliographies Online is available by subscription and perpetual access to institutions. For more information or to contact an Oxford Sales Representative click here.
Article
- Acceptability Judgments
- Accessibility Theory in Linguistics
- Acquisition, Second Language, and Bilingualism, Psycholin...
- Adjectives
- Adpositions
- Affixation
- African Linguistics
- Afroasiatic Languages
- Agreement
- Algonquian Linguistics
- Altaic Languages
- Ambiguity, Lexical
- Analogy in Language and Linguistics
- Anaphora
- Animal Communication
- Aphasia
- Applicatives
- Applied Linguistics, Critical
- Arawak Languages
- Argument Structure
- Artificial Languages
- Attention and Salience
- Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Dyslexia in A...
- Australian Languages
- Austronesian Linguistics
- Auxiliaries
- Balkans, The Languages of the
- Baudouin de Courtenay, Jan
- Berber Languages and Linguistics
- Bilingualism and Multilingualism
- Biology of Language
- Blocking
- Borrowing, Structural
- Caddoan Languages
- Caucasian Languages
- Causatives
- Celtic Languages
- Celtic Mutations
- Chomsky, Noam
- Chumashan Languages
- Classifiers
- Clauses, Relative
- Clinical Linguistics
- Cognitive Linguistics
- Colonial Place Names
- Comparative Reconstruction in Linguistics
- Comparative-Historical Linguistics
- Complementation
- Complexity, Linguistic
- Compositionality
- Compounding
- Comprehension, Sentence
- Computational Linguistics
- Conditionals
- Conjunctions
- Connectionism
- Consonant Epenthesis
- Constructions, Verb-Particle
- Contrastive Analysis in Linguistics
- Conversation Analysis
- Conversation, Maxims of
- Conversational Implicature
- Cooperative Principle
- Coordination
- Copula
- Creoles
- Creoles, Grammatical Categories in
- Critical Periods
- Cross-Language Speech Perception and Production
- Cyberpragmatics
- Default Semantics
- Definiteness
- Dementia and Language
- Dene (Athabaskan) Languages
- Dené-Yeniseian Hypothesis, The
- Dependencies
- Dependencies, Long Distance
- Derivational Morphology
- Determiners
- Dialectology
- Dialogue
- Diglossia
- Disfluency
- Distinctive Features
- Dravidian Languages
- Ellipsis
- Endangered Languages
- English as a Lingua Franca
- English, Early Modern
- English, Old
- Ergativity
- Eskimo-Aleut
- Euphemisms and Dysphemisms
- Evidentials
- Exemplar-Based Models in Linguistics
- Existential
- Existential Wh-Constructions
- Experimental Linguistics
- Fieldwork
- Fieldwork, Sociolinguistic
- Finite State Languages
- First Language Attrition
- Formulaic Language
- Francoprovençal
- French Grammars
- Frisian
- Gabelentz, Georg von der
- Gender
- Genealogical Classification
- Generative Syntax
- Genetics and Language
- Gestures
- Grammar, Categorial
- Grammar, Cognitive
- Grammar, Construction
- Grammar, Descriptive
- Grammar, Functional Discourse
- Grammars, Phrase Structure
- Grammaticalization
- Harris, Zellig
- Heritage Languages
- History of Linguistics
- History of the English Language
- Hmong-Mien Languages
- Hokan Languages
- Honorifics
- Humor in Language
- Hungarian Vowel Harmony
- Iconicity
- Ideophones
- Idiolect
- Idiom and Phraseology
- Imperatives
- Indefiniteness
- Indo-European Etymology
- Inflected Infinitives
- Information Structure
- Innateness
- Interface Between Phonology and Phonetics
- Interjections
- Intonation
- IPA
- Irony
- Iroquoian Languages
- Islands
- Isolates, Language
- Jakobson, Roman
- Japanese Word Accent
- Jones, Daniel
- Juncture and Boundary
- Khoisan Languages
- Kiowa-Tanoan Languages
- Kra-Dai Languages
- Labov, William
- Language Acquisition
- Language and Law
- Language Contact
- Language Documentation
- Language, Embodiment and
- Language for Specific Purposes/Specialized Communication
- Language, Gender, and Sexuality
- Language Geography
- Language Ideologies and Language Attitudes
- Language in Autism Spectrum Disorders
- Language Nests
- Language Revitalization
- Language Shift
- Language Standardization
- Language, Synesthesia and
- Languages of Africa
- Languages of the Americas, Indigenous
- Languages of the World
- Learnability
- Lexemes
- Lexical Access, Cognitive Mechanisms for
- Lexical Semantics
- Lexical-Functional Grammar
- Lexicography
- Lexicography, Bilingual
- Lexicon
- Linguistic Accommodation
- Linguistic Anthropology
- Linguistic Areas
- Linguistic Landscapes
- Linguistic Prescriptivism
- Linguistic Profiling and Language-Based Discrimination
- Linguistic Relativity
- Linguistics, Educational
- Listening, Second Language
- Literature and Linguistics
- Loanwords
- Machine Translation
- Maintenance, Language
- Mande Languages
- Markedness
- Mass-Count Distinction
- Mathematical Linguistics
- Mayan Languages
- Mental Health Disorders, Language in
- Mental Lexicon, The
- Mesoamerican Languages
- Metaphor
- Metathesis
- Metonymy
- Minority Languages
- Mixed Languages
- Mixe-Zoquean Languages
- Modification
- Mon-Khmer Languages
- Morphological Change
- Morphology
- Morphology, Blending in
- Morphology, Subtractive
- Movement
- Munda Languages
- Muskogean Languages
- Nasals and Nasalization
- Negation
- Niger-Congo Languages
- Non-Pama-Nyungan Languages
- Northeast Caucasian Languages
- Nostratic
- Number
- Numerals
- Oceanic Languages
- Papuan Languages
- Penutian Languages
- Philosophy of Language
- Phonetics
- Phonetics, Acoustic
- Phonetics, Articulatory
- Phonological Research, Psycholinguistic Methodology in
- Phonology
- Phonology, Computational
- Phonology, Early Child
- Pidgins
- Polarity
- Policy and Planning, Language
- Politeness in Language
- Polysemy
- Positive Discourse Analysis
- Possessives, Acquisition of
- Pragmatics, Acquisition of
- Pragmatics, Cognitive
- Pragmatics, Computational
- Pragmatics, Cross-Cultural
- Pragmatics, Developmental
- Pragmatics, Experimental
- Pragmatics, Game Theory in
- Pragmatics, Historical
- Pragmatics, Institutional
- Pragmatics, Second Language
- Pragmatics, Teaching
- Prague Linguistic Circle, The
- Presupposition
- Pronouns
- Psycholinguistics
- Quechuan and Aymaran Languages
- Questions
- Reading, Second-Language
- Reciprocals
- Reduplication
- Reflexives and Reflexivity
- Register and Register Variation
- Relevance Theory
- Representation and Processing of Multi-Word Expressions in...
- Salish Languages
- Sapir, Edward
- Saussure, Ferdinand de
- Second Language Acquisition, Anaphora Resolution in
- Semantic Maps
- Semantic Roles
- Semantic-Pragmatic Change
- Semantics, Cognitive
- Sentence Processing in Monolingual and Bilingual Speakers
- Sign Language Linguistics
- Slang
- Sociolinguistics
- Sociolinguistics, Variationist
- Sociopragmatics
- Sonority
- Sound Change
- South American Indian Languages
- Specific Language Impairment
- Speech, Deceptive
- Speech Perception
- Speech Production
- Speech Synthesis
- Suppletion
- Switch-Reference
- Syllables
- Syncretism
- Synonymy
- Syntactic Change
- Syntactic Knowledge, Children’s Acquisition of
- Tense, Aspect, and Mood
- Text Mining
- Tone
- Tone Sandhi
- Topic
- Transcription
- Transitivity and Voice
- Translanguaging
- Translation
- Trubetzkoy, Nikolai
- Tucanoan Languages
- Tupian Languages
- Typology
- Usage-Based Linguistics
- Uto-Aztecan Languages
- Valency Theory
- Verbs, Serial
- Vocabulary, Second Language
- Voice and Voice Quality
- Vowel Harmony
- Whitney, William Dwight
- Word Classes
- Word Formation in Japanese
- Word Recognition, Spoken
- Word Recognition, Visual
- Word Stress
- Writing, Second Language
- Writing Systems
- Yiddish
- Zapotecan Languages