In This Article Expand or collapse the "in this article" section The Weapon Focus Effect in Eyewitness Memory

  • Introduction
  • Overview: Scientific Status of the Weapon Focus Effect
  • The Basics of Weapon Focus
  • The Multiple Drivers of the Weapon Focus Effect
  • Threat and Arousal
  • Threat Without Arousal
  • Context
  • Unusualness (Novelty)
  • The Weapon Focus Effect Among Police Officers
  • The Weapon Focus Effect with Children
  • Juror Knowledge of the Weapon Focus Effect
  • Ancillary Tests of the Weapon Focus Effect
  • Weapon Focus in Real Eyewitness Events

Psychology The Weapon Focus Effect in Eyewitness Memory
by
Nancy K. Steblay
  • LAST MODIFIED: 26 May 2023
  • DOI: 10.1093/obo/9780199828340-0313

Introduction

The identification of criminal perpetrators by eyewitnesses is a staple form of evidence in courts of law. In the 1970s psychological scientists began a systematic study of eyewitness identification using controlled experiments, and much has been learned through thousands of lab studies about factors that put the reliability of eyewitness evidence at risk. Laboratory science has soundly established that the rate of mistaken identification varies as a function of conditions during the witnessed event, the retention interval, and the method of conducting an identification procedure. A useful distinction has been made between two types of factors that influence eyewitness accuracy: system and estimator variables. System variables are those that are (or could be) controlled to improve the accuracy of eyewitness reports, particularly through science-informed police procedures such as non-leading interview protocols, fair lineup structure, and double-blind lineups. Estimator variables influence eyewitness memory and accuracy but are not under the control of the criminal justice system, such as conditions during the crime event (e.g., lighting, distance, obstacles to view), aspects of the criminal (e.g., number of perpetrators, facial disguise), and characteristics of the witness (e.g., visual acuity, stress). One estimator variable—referred to as the weapon focus effect—describes the phenomenon in which the presence of a weapon at the scene of a crime impairs the witness’s memory for the perpetrator and other details of the crime. The basic idea is that the weapon captures the visual attention of the witness and thereby distracts witness attention from other important features of the visual scene such as the face of the perpetrator. The eyewitness is simply less able to adequately encode and subsequently recall details peripheral to the weapon. The weapon focus effect has garnered attention in labs and courtrooms over the more than forty years of eyewitness research. The cumulative body of weapon focus research provides support for the claim that the presence of a weapon draws the attention of an eyewitness. In doing so, this weapon focus can diminish the ability of the eyewitness to later recognize and describe the culprit.

Overview: Scientific Status of the Weapon Focus Effect

Kassin, et al. 1989 assessed the status of a variety of eyewitness memory topics through a survey of eyewitness experts. A number of topics were endorsed as strongly established principles (e.g., regarding biased lineup instructions), but only 57 percent of experts agreed that the weapon focus effect was reliable enough for psychologists to present in courtroom testimony. This report served as a catalyst for a meta-analytic review of the nascent weapon focus literature. Steblay 1992 located nineteen tests of the weapon focus effect from twelve articles and reported a significant overall difference between weapon-present and weapon-absent conditions, with a small effect for witnesses’ lineup identification accuracy, and a moderate impairment for witnesses’ memory of perpetrator clothing and facial characteristics (“feature accuracy”). Witness accuracy was worse in a weapon-present condition compared to a weapon-absent condition. A decade later, Kassin, et al. 2001 repeated the survey of experts, and the majority of eyewitness scientists at that point were convinced of the reliability of the weapon focus effect (WFE), a significant jump from 57 percent to 87 percent. Fawcett, et al. 2013 followed with a meta-analysis of studies to 2009. Their review supported the weapon focus effect, again noting a small impact of the weapon on witness identification accuracy and a larger impact on feature accuracy. In 2014, the National Academy of Sciences (NAS) confirmed that “the so-called weapon focus is a real-world case in point for eyewitness identification, in which attention is compellingly drawn to emotionally laden stimuli, such as a gun or a knife, at the expense of acquiring greater visual information about the face of the perpetrator” (National Academy of Sciences 2014, p. 55). Fawcett, et al. 2016 followed up the 2013 review with a summary article that encompassed previous work on the topic, including three prior meta-analyses (Steblay 1992; Fawcett, et al. 2013; Kocab and Sporer 2016). They concluded that laboratory evidence demonstrates that an unexpected weapon reduces the accuracy of subsequent suspect identification attempts and witness descriptions of the crime: “The weapon focus effect is sufficiently robust and uncontroversial to guide practice so long as consideration is given to the circumstances surrounding the criminal event with a particular emphasis on witness expectation” (p. 257).

  • Fawcett, J. M., K. A. Peace, and A. Greve. 2016. Looking down the barrel of a gun: What do we know about the weapon focus effect? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 5.3: 257–263.

    DOI: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.07.005

    Fawcett and colleagues state support for the WFE but also caution regarding evaluation in real crime scenarios. Three factors may influence the impact of the WFE: a witness’s perceived threat; aspects of the crime (e.g., duration), and the memory collection procedure. They conclude that the weapon focus is appropriate for court consideration, but must be scrutinized to determine its relevance in a given case.

  • Fawcett, J. M., E. J. Russell, K. A. Peace, and J. Christie. 2013. Of guns and geese: A meta-analytic review of the “weapon focus” literature. Psychology, Crime, and Law 19.1: 35–66.

    DOI: 10.1080/1068316X.2011.599325

    This meta-analysis supported the WFE for eyewitness identification accuracy (g =.22) and feature accuracy (g =.75). A rule of thumb is that g =.2 is a small effect size, g =.5 is a medium effect size, and g =.8 is a large effect size.

  • Kassin, S., P. C. Ellsworth, and V. L. Smith. 1989. The general acceptance of psychological research on eyewitness testimony: A survey of the experts. American Psychologist 44:1089–1098.

    DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.8.1089

    Sixty-three experts were surveyed about their courtroom experiences and opinions on eyewitness topics. Just 56.5 percent of the experts judged the research on weapon focus to be reliable enough to testify about it; 27 percent reported that they had testified about weapon focus.

  • Kassin, S. M., V. A. Tubb, H. M. Hosch, and A. Memon. 2001. On the “general acceptance” of eyewitness testimony research: A new survey of the experts. American Psychologist 56: 405–416.

    DOI: 10.1037//0003-.66X.56.5.405

    Sixty-four experts were asked about their experiences and opinions on thirty eyewitness phenomena. The weapon focus effect was rated reliable by 87 percent of the experts; 77 percent stated that they would testify on this topic. Ninety-seven percent of experts agreed that there is a research basis for testimony.

  • Kocab, K., and S. Sporer. 2016. The weapon focus effect for person identifications and descriptions: A meta-analysis. In Advances in psychology and law. Edited by M. K. Miller and B. H. Bornstein, 71–117. New York: Springer.

    This chapter is published in an edited volume. The review covered studies through 2009. Analysis found a moderate effect size for feature accuracy and for description of the weapon, but not a significant WFE for identification accuracy. The Fawcett, et al. 2016 statement considered this work as part of its conclusion that the WFE is sufficiently robust to be considered in court.

  • National Academy of Sciences. 2014. Identifying the culprit: Assessing eyewitness identification. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

    A committee of the National Academy of Sciences reviewed eyewitness science and published their recommendations for use of eyewitness science in practice and policy. The report summarizes the science of estimator and system variables.

  • Steblay, N. M. 1992. A meta-analytic review of the weapon-focus effect. Law and Human Behavior 16:413–424.

    DOI: 10.1007/BF02352267

    This first meta-analysis of the WFE reported a small but statistically significant effect size (h =.13) for identification accuracy of witnesses (reduced accuracy when the weapon was present versus absent) and a moderate effect size (d =.55) for feature accuracy. The report included twelve articles, of which nine were published.

  • Wells, G. L. 1978. Applied eyewitness testimony research: System variables and estimator variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36:1546–1557.

    DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.36.12.1546

    A seminal article for eyewitness research that provided a framework for understanding how two different types of factors influence eyewitness accuracy: estimator and system variables. The weapon focus effect is one of many estimator variables that have been explored in research studies.

  • Wells, G. L., M. B. Kovera, A. B. Douglass, N. Brewer, C. A. Meissner, and J. T. Wixted. 2020. Policy and procedure recommendations for the collection and preservation of eyewitness identification evidence. Law and Human Behavior 44.1: 3–36.

    DOI: 10.1037/lhb0000359

    This is a consensus document of eyewitness scientists stating nine recommendations for the collection of identification evidence from eyewitnesses. This document summarizes much of the research on system variables.

back to top

Users without a subscription are not able to see the full content on this page. Please subscribe or login.

How to Subscribe

Oxford Bibliographies Online is available by subscription and perpetual access to institutions. For more information or to contact an Oxford Sales Representative click here.

Article

Up

Down